Delusion-proneness predicts COVID-19 vaccination behavior

1 Introduction

Vaccination-related conspiracy ideation can reduce an individual’s likelihood to comply with public health campaigns, which aim to reduce interpersonal transmission of COVID-19 (1, 2). Studies have shown that misinformation and conspiracy ideation impact vaccination intentions negatively (3, 4). Despite implemented efforts, approximately 25% of the EU population has not been vaccinated for COVID-19 as of today (5). The causes to why people avoid getting vaccinated are poorly understood, even though a role of certain psychological characteristics and personality traits have been suggested to be related to vaccination hesitancy (68).

Research suggests that individuals scoring higher on delusion-proneness, a personality trait in the normal population associated with overvalued and delusion-like beliefs (9, 10), are more likely to develop conspiracy beliefs related to the COVID-19 pandemic and the associated vaccination program (7, 11, 12). While several of these studies were cross sectional, delusion proneness was measured before the out-break of the COVID-19 pandemic in one study (7), suggesting a possible causal link. In addition to the delusion-proneness personality trait, cognitive biases associated with delusional ideation such as Bias Against Disconfirmatory Evidence (BADE) and Jumping to Conclusions (JC) may also play a role in the development of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs (7, 11). Finally, conjunction fallacy, i.e. biases in probabilistic reasoning whereby people overestimate the likelihood of co-occurring events, have been suggested to be related to conspiracy ideation, including COVID-19 conspiracy ideation (13, 14). Thus, it is already well-established that delusion proneness and related style of information processing is associated with development of COVID-19 conspiracy ideas. However, it is not known how development of such conspiracy ideas affects vaccination status, which is the main aim of the present study.

While the delusion-proneness trait may be a driving factor for not getting vaccinated, there are potentially other psychiatric traits that may have a negative impact on vaccination rates (15, 16). For example, anxiety may influence the willingness to get vaccinated in several ways. It is possible that anxious individuals are more likely to get vaccinated due to worry about the complications related to COVID-19, but such individuals may also choose not to get vaccinated due to concerns about the possible adverse effects of the vaccination. To date, it is not clearly described which of these two behavioral pathways are more likely although anxiety has been shown to be associated with a more positive attitude toward vaccination in one study (17). On the other hand, anxiety has also been shown to drive conspiracy beliefs generally (18), as well as COVID-19 conspiracies specifically (19). Other studies have also found that external threats (such as the COVID-19 pandemic) trigger the behavioral inhibition system (BIS), associated with heightened vigilance and anxiety, which in turn activates distal defenses such as increased beliefs in conspiracy theories (20). Apart from trait-anxiety, several other psychiatric traits may impact vaccination behavior. For example, as there is a link between both Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) traits with delusion-proneness (21), they may also impact decisions making related to vaccination. In line with this reasoning, previous evidence suggest that ASD traits are associated with increased conspiracy ideation (22). As ADHD- and ASD-traits are closely associated to delusion-proneness (21) and previous studies have suggested that anxiety may impact vaccination behavior, adjusting for them is of importance in order to understand whether delusion-proneness specifically relates to not getting vaccinated. Measuring these traits would also give a better insight into which psychiatric traits drive choice to vaccinate, apart from delusion-proneness and how much each trait contributes to this choice.

In summary, the general association between delusion-proneness and COVID-19 conspiracy ideas is already established (7, 11, 12). In the present study, we went one step further and tested whether delusion-proneness also is specifically linked to the vaccination rate both in terms of the outcome (vaccinated or not) and how long it takes to get vaccinated from when the vaccination program started. Also, we implemented a path analysis, in order to better describe whether the delusion proneness may be related to vaccination behavior through development of COVID-19 conspiracy ideas. Finally, in order to better understand the motives for not choosing (or choosing) to vaccinate we also performed an exploratory analysis focusing on how the subjects reasoned about their choice using a text analytic tool.

2 Materials and methods2.1 Subjects and general study design

We invited subjects from our previous studies on delusion-proneness and conspiracy ideation (7, 23) to complete a follow-up survey which consisted of questions pertaining to whether or not they have been vaccinated against COVID-19, the date for when the first dose was received, as well as to freely express in text why they chose to vaccinate or not. They also completed the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-T) to assess their trait anxiety level (24). Previously, they had completed the Ritvo Autism Asperger Diagnostic Scale–Revised (RAADS) (25) to assess ASD traits and the adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS) (26) to assess ADHD-traits. The subjects were invited to complete the follow-up survey via email which was sent out in October 2021. Out of the 1032 invited subjects, 273 completed the follow-up survey. See Supplementary Materials for further details on the sampling procedure and Figure 1 for a timeline of the current and previous study on the relation between delusion proneness and development of conspiracy ideation (7). Note, that the initial study from where we invited subjects was over-sampled for delusion proneness and use of psychedelics (23). Thus, the present population does not represent a general population in terms of delusion proneness distribution.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Timeline showing dates of data sampling for the study where the relation between delusion proneness and later development of COVID-19 conspiracy believes was studied (7) and present study, including important dates pertaining to the COVID-19 pandemic.

The range of age for the whole sample was 18-67 years (N = 273, M = 29.6, SD = 7.21), education (years of University studies or equivalent) was 0-12 years (M = 3.37, SD = 2.50), and mean score for PDI was 3.93 (SD = 2.99, N = 268). 197 (73.5%) of the 273 who answered the follow-up survey were female, whereas 71 (26.5%) were male. 36 (13.19% had not been vaccinated against COVID-19, and 237 (86.81%) had received a vaccine. 189 (70.5%) had not been diagnosed with a psychiatric disorder (major depressive disorder, psychotic disorder, obsessive compulsive disorder, ASD, ADHD, post-traumatic stress disorder and anxiety disorder), while 79 (29.5%) had been diagnosed with a psychiatric disorder. See Table 1 below for more detail.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Showing descriptive data of our sample (N = 273).

The study was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (27), and approved by the Swedish Ethical Review Authority. All subjects gave informed consent prior to answering the survey. The main outcome analyses were performed adhering to a preregistered protocol (https://osf.io/npb8q/wiki/home).

2.2 Quantification and statistical analysis

To test differences in delusion-proneness (using PDI) between vaccinated and non-vaccinated groups we first conducted a Wilcoxon test due to non-normally distributed data. Similarly, we conducted a Spearman’s correlation to test correlation between PDI and time to vaccination.

We then applied a series of logistic regression analyses to investigate the association between COVID-19 vaccination status (either received or not) and a set of predictor variables. Starting with a base model, we sequentially incorporated relevant covariates across different models, such as ASRS, RAADS, STAI-T, psychiatric diagnoses, sex, and education, to adjust for potential confounders. Each regression model yielded beta weights, standard errors, z-values, p-values, and odds ratios to interpret the relationship between predictors and the likelihood of getting vaccinated against COVID-19.

Subsequently, we conducted linear regression analyses, where the response variable was the time taken to receive the vaccine. Again, various models were designed by adding the different predictors and covariates to elucidate their influence on the time to vaccination. Beta coefficients, t-values, and p-values were computed to gauge the significance and strength of these relationships.

Finally, a path analysis was carried out to discern direct and mediated effects of the predictors on the decision to get vaccinated. The analysis specifically explored how PDI impacts the decision to vaccinate, both directly and through its effect on COVID-19 Conspiracy Questionnaire (CCQ) Total – (see Supplementary Material). All models were scrutinized using appropriate statistical criteria to ascertain their validity and reliability (see Supplementary Material).

2.3 Exploratory analyses2.3.1 Word cloud and text analysis

We performed text analysis of the open question on why or why not the subjects had taken the vaccine. We used the “bing” sentiment lexicon (28) in the R programming language (29). We then created a word cloud showing the most common positive and negative words used by each group. Furthermore, we classified each subject’s comments based on the overarching theme that were identified and created a bar-chart showing the average frequency of themes in each group.

3 Results3.1 Group comparison and correlational analysis

The first step of the analyses was focused on group comparisons (Figure 2A), which showed that unvaccinated individuals scored higher on delusion-proneness (W=2225.5, p<0.001, Wilcoxon's effect size =0.27). Notably, the scoring of delusion-proneness was performed before the launch of the vaccination program against COVID-19. For the subset of subjects that got vaccinated against COVID-19 (N = 237), we conducted a Spearman’s correlation which showed a positive correlation between PDI and time to get vaccinated (rs=0.27, p<.001), see Figure 2B.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Boxplot showing the comparison of means of PDI between unvaccinated and vaccinated subjects. Each data point represents an individual. (B) Scatter plot showing the relationship between PDI and time to get vaccinate (in months) from when the vaccination program started. The cloud represents accumulated effect of overlapping data points, while black dots represent single data points with jitter. The darker shade of blue represents more data points (N =229), *** p <.001. PDI: Peters et al. Delusions Inventory. rw = Wilcoxon's effect size. rs = Spearman's correlation.

3.2 Logistic regression

We performed a series of logistic regression models to analyze how well PDI predicted being vaccinated when potential confounders were added to the model. In all models a higher PDI predicted a reduced likelihood to vaccinate. The predictive power of PDI was only marginally affected when adding other psychiatric traits (Model 2), psychiatric diagnoses (Model 3), education and sex to the models (Model 4) which all are potential confounders (Table 2). Of interest, both psychiatric diagnosis and anxiety also predicted whether individuals got vaccinated, but in opposite ways (psychiatric diagnoses were associated with lower degree of vaccination, anxiety was associated with higher degree of vaccination). Testing for multicollinearity showed that minimal variance inflation factor remains for the logistic regression models (1.03-1.37), comparison of AIC of the logistic models showed that Model 4 fit the data better, see Table 1 for more details.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Likelihood to get vaccinated against COVID-19 (vaccinated vs non-vaccinated).

3.3 Linear regression

Next, four linear regression analyses were carried out to analyze what factors were associated with the time to get vaccinated (in months) in the group that had been vaccinated. For each model, predictors were added in a step-wise fashion in order to adjust for the effects of covariates. Similar to the results of the logistic regression for the binary outcome (vaccinated or not vaccinated), PDI was a stable predictor of the days it took to get vaccinated (Table 3). Of interest, both education and sex impacted how long time it took to getting vaccinated. While sex was associated with a longer time to get vaccinated, i.e. women took longer time to get vaccinated, education was associated with a somewhat quicker time to get vaccinated. Testing for multicollinearity showed that minimal variance inflation factor remains for the linear regression models (1.03-1.38), while comparison of AIC of the linear models showed that Model 4 had the best fit, see Table 2 for further details on model comparison.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Standardized linear effects of predictors on time to vaccination against COVID-19.

3.4 Adjustment for age

Adjusting for age was not straightforward as different age groups were offered to start vaccination at different periods, i.e. age was cofounded by possibility to get vaccinated. Therefore, we did not adjust for age in our main analyses, but treated age and time offered to vaccinate as noise in the data (adhering to the preregistration protocol). However, we also performed two sensitivity analyses to reduce the effect from these factors. We first conducted the same logistic and linear regression analyses but only with young adults between 18-35 years, who were offered vaccination at the same time, showing similar results. We also conducted the same logistic and linear regression analyses using all subjects where we adjusted for age. These analyses also showed similar results as the main analyses (see Supplementary Tables S1–S4). These results suggested that our main analyses were not confounded by age. As only 27 subjects were above 35 years, it was not possible to study whether different age groups differed in when they chose to get vaccinated.

3.5 Path analysis

A path analysis using r package (lavaan) with the Sobel test was performed to examine the mediating role of COVID-19 Conspiracy Questionnaire (CCQ)6 between PDI and vaccination against COVID-19. We found that CCQ partially mediates the relationship between PDI and vaccination (β = -0.18, z = -3.17, p = .002). See Figure 3 and Supplementary Materials for details.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Path analyses (with standardized parameter values) of how delusion-proneness (measured with PDI) predicts vaccination with COVID-19 Conspiracy ideation as a mediator. PDI = Peters et al. Delusions Inventory; CCQ Total = total score on the COVID-19 Conspiracy Questionnaire. ** p <.01, *** p <.001.

3.6 Exploratory analyses3.6.1 Interaction analysis

As we observed a main effect for both delusion-proneness and anxiety in our main analyses we examined the relationship between delusion-proneness, anxiety and vaccination against COVID-19, performing a logistic regression with an interaction term. The interaction showed that it was particularly individuals with both low anxiety and high delusion-proneness that were less likely to get vaccinated (N = 273, Z = 3.588, p=0.003) (Supplementary Figure S1).

3.7 Text analysis

Subjects were instructed to explain why they had taken a COVID-19 vaccine or not, in writing. The responses were text-analyzed to extract most commonly occurring words and sentiments from the full written text. Lastly, we classified every subject’s comment on why they did or did not get vaccinated against COVID-19 into themes (see Figures 4A, B).

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) Word cloud showing most frequent words used by unvaccinated subjects (left) and for vaccinated subjects (right). The bigger words are more frequently used than the smaller ones. Results from the text analysis suggested that unvaccinated individuals used more negative words and themes regarding the vaccination while vaccinated individuals used more positive words about protecting themselves and other individuals. Thus, while both groups were concerned about safety issues, conceptualizations of safety differed between them. A group comparison was performed in order to test if the average number of words used was significantly different between vaccinated and unvaccinated subjects and found that on average, unvaccinated subjects used more words (mean=52.03, SD=83.67, median=20.5, range 2-422) to describe why they did not get vaccinated compared to the vaccinated subjects (average=14.74, SD=10.92, median=11, range 2-61) (W=989.9, p=.002). This result remained significant even after excluding two extreme cases in the non-vaccinated group (W=989.5, p=.009). We then conducted a correlation analysis and found no significant correlation between number of words and PDI (r=-.22, p=0.22) or CCQ (r=-.09, p=0.62). (B) Average frequency of themes found in subject’s responses on why they did or did not get vaccinated (unvaccinated n = 36, vaccinated n = 237). Note that every subject’s response was tested for all identified themes.

4 Discussion

While several studies have suggested that conspiracy ideation relates to lower intention to get vaccinated (3033), the present study expands on the previous results in showing that delusion-proneness and conspiracy ideation was linked to whether or not individuals also got vaccinated, as well as how long time it took to get vaccinated in the vaccinated group. Namely, individuals who were not vaccinated against COVID-19 scored significantly higher on the trait delusion-proneness measured by PDI. Additionally, we showed a positive correlation between PDI and time to get vaccinated in the vaccinated group. These effects were robust and remained after adjusting for demographic factors, psychiatric diagnoses, and psychiatric traits relating to anxiety, ASD, and ADHD as well as age.

It is important to consider confounds from other psychiatric traits as previous studies have shown a comorbidity between delusion-proneness, ADHD- and ASD traits (21). Our study suggests that specifically delusion-proneness is associated with the development of these conspiracy theories and ultimately a hesitancy against vaccination. Another trait identified as an important contributor to the outcome was anxiety, as anxiety has been previously associated with a more positive attitude toward vaccination (17), which is in line with our finding showing that subjects with high trait-anxiety are more likely to vaccinate against COVID-19. However, we did not find an effect of trait anxiety in our time analyses. Interestingly, we observed an interaction effect showing that subjects with low anxiety and high delusion-proneness were less likely to get vaccinated against COVID-19, compared to high anxiety and high delusion-prone individuals. ASD and ADHD-traits did not have any association with vaccination behavior in our study. Thus, to our knowledge our results are the first to show that delusion-proneness is a significant factor explaining some of the variance in vaccination behavior, and remains significant when controlling for other major psychiatric traits that suggest no negative impact on vaccination. In order to better understand the relative importance of delusion-proneness on vaccination behavior, future studies should include a broader range of personality traits and traits associated with psychiatric disorders.

It should be noted that delusion-proneness is a personality trait observed as a continuum in the population (10). While individuals exhibiting high levels of delusion-proneness may share certain cognitive biases with psychotic disorders (3437), it is important to avoid pathologizing this personality trait that often is associated with a normal, and sometimes even favorable, functioning (38, 39).

The present project is longitudinal in that it follows the development of conspiracy ideation and their later consequences over a period of almost three years (Figure 1, Supplementary Materials). As we measured delusion proneness 2018 and 2019, before the COVID-19 pandemic, and then we followed the group into the pandemic, we could study how conspiracy beliefs were developed 2020 in relation to previously established delusion proneness and associated cognitive biases (7). The present study is based on the third measurement on this population, focusing on how delusion proneness and development of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs is associated with later vaccination behavior (measured autumn 2021). A key question is why delusion proneness trait is related to development of conspiracy ideas that determines later behavior? Theoretical considerations have suggested that the psychosis phenotype is associated with an imbalance in the hierarchical predictive coding system of the brain with weak and unprecise low level priors, leading to development of multiple error signals (on an information processing level) and experience of aberrant salience (on a phenomenological level) (40, 41). Such aberrant salience calls for novel explanations that can incorporate them into a coherent explanation of the world, triggering the development of delusional ideas (40, 41). Thus, it may be suggested that development of conspiracy ideas is a consequence of the cognitive style of information processing associated with delusion-proneness. This mechanism suggests that a key component in decreasing vaccination hesitancy would be based on how information is given.

As it took longer to get vaccinated for those who scored high on delusion-proneness, such individuals may require more information before taking the final decision to vaccinate. This finding suggests that under the right circumstances, individuals with high delusion-proneness may accept vaccination. For example, individuals with high delusion-proneness may eventually be persuaded to get vaccinated when information is communicated through different channels, and addressing their safety concerns.

We also found that it took longer time for women to get vaccinated compared to men. This is in line with a recently published systematic review and meta-analysis (42), showing that women in the general population, especially women who work in healthcare have less intentions to get vaccinated against COVID-19 than men.

In the study, measurement of delusion-proneness was performed before the COVID-19 pandemic, whereas measurement of COVID-19 conspiracy ideation was performed during the pandemic, and the assessment of whether individuals had vaccinated or not was performed in a third step. Our path analysis supports that subjects with high delusion-proneness acquire COVID-19 conspiracy ideas more often than others, which increases the risk to avoid vaccinations. However, previous research suggests that the opposite is possible as well, i.e., intentions to not get vaccinated may in fact strengthen and develop conspiracy ideation (32). While such reverse causation is possible for the development of specific conspiracy ideas, it is likely that basic aspects of delusion-proneness predict how novel beliefs are acquired.

Our text analysis for the reasons the subjects got vaccinated or not, showed that unvaccinated subjects tended to use more negative words and themes regarding the vaccine, such as its effectiveness, lack of transparency as well as that it is unsafe and that they are afraid of the side effects. For vaccinated subjects, we observed more negative words and themes directed at the virus and the disease, whereas the positive words were related to protection of self and others, solidarity and that it is self-evident to take the vaccine to stop the spread. In line with this, unvaccinated individuals used more words in general, possibly suggesting a stronger desire to justify their decision. This could be related to that unvaccinated people reason why they did not get vaccinated, while vaccinated people reason why they did get vaccinated.

There are a number of limitations in the present study. Although the used metrics leveraged in the present study (scores on PDI) have been shown to have a good test-retest reliability (9, 26, 43) our study measures are self-rated, which limits the objectivity of the evaluations. Another limitation of the present study is that somatic diseases or other psychiatric and personality traits than the ones measured here, may have an impact on willingness to get vaccinated. Also, while we cannot exclude selection bias in this study, the distribution of PDI scores was not suggestive of selection bias within this dimension. However, as there was an oversampling of high delusion proneness in the initial study (23) from where we invited the subjects to participate in the present study, we cannot directly infer our results to a general population. Furthermore, education was only measured as numbers of years of university education (or similar) as a large majority of Sweden’s population complete secondary education. Finally, another limitation of our study is the small sample size which one needs to consider when interpreting the results presented in the paper. The main strength of the study is that it follows a cohort from before the COVID-19 pandemic, over the pandemic and until a vaccination has been available for everyone. This gives a unique opportunity to study formation of conspiracy beliefs and the later behavioral consequences.

In summary, our results show that individuals with higher levels of delusion-proneness are more reluctant to get vaccinated against COVID-19, and that this is not only driven by the general cognitive style of delusion-prone trait phenotype, but also by development of beliefs in COVID-19 conspiracy theories. Importantly, of the individuals with high delusion-proneness that vaccinated, it took longer time to get vaccinated after vaccines were available. On a more general level, the present findings suggest that a reluctance to get vaccinated may represent an example of how neurocognitive and psychiatric traits may impact real-life behaviors with direct implications for public health and society. Our results may also have implications and give input for vaccinations programs for children, where vaccine rates have been shown to be dropping (44). Future studies should aim to investigate how providing more tailored information and public campaigns, related to an individual’s vaccination and safety concerns, can be implemented to better support people in regard to their decision-making processes related to pandemics.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: https://github.com/kasimacar/CCQOnlineBADE.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Etikprövningsnämnden, Stockholm Sweden. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

KA: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation. AK: Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Investigation. AO: Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Funding acquisition. J-WP: Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LB: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JA: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MI: Funding acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AL: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. PP: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. AL was supported by Swedish Research Foundation Consolidator Grant (2018-00877). JA was supported by grants from Riksbankens Jubileumsfond (P18-0523). LB was supported by grants from SU – Region Stockholm (FoUI-980356). PP was supported by grants from the Swedish Research Foundation (Vetenskapsrådet; 2014-30186-113005-19 and 2019-01253), ALF Medicine (20140306 and 20160039), Karolinska Institutet (2-70/2014-97; KID-funding 2011; KID-funding 2020), Riksbankens Jubileumsfond (P18-0523), Hjärnfonden, and Marianne och Marcus Wallenbergs Stiftelse (MMW2014.0065).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1450429/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Borges Do Nascimento IJ, Pizarro AB, Almeida JM, Azzopardi-Muscat N, Goncalves MA, Bjorklund M, et al. Infodemics and health misinformation: a systematic review of reviews. Bull World Health Organ. (2022) 100:544–61. doi: 10.2471/BLT.21.287654

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Bruns H, Dessart FJ, Pantazi M. Covid-19 misinformation: preparing for future crises. Luxembourg: EUR 31139 EN, Publications Office of the European Union (2022). doi: 10.2760/41905,JRC130111

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Loomba S, De Figueiredo A, Piatek SJ, De Graaf K, Larson HJ. Measuring the impact of COVID-19 vaccine misinformation on vaccination intent in the UK and USA. Nat Hum Behav. (2021) 5:337–48. doi: 10.1038/s41562-021-01056-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Yanto TA, Octavius GS, Heriyanto RS, Ienawi C, Nisa H, Pasai HE. Psychological factors affecting COVID-19 vaccine acceptance in Indonesia. Egypt J Neurol Psychiatr Neurosurg. (2021) 57:177. doi: 10.1186/s41983-021-00436-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Acar K, Horntvedt O, Cabrera A, Olsson A, Ingvar M, Lebedev AV, et al. COVID-19 conspiracy ideation is associated with the delusion proneness trait and resistance to update of beliefs. Sci Rep. (2022) 12:10352. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-14071-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Halstead IN, Mckay RT, Lewis GJ. COVID-19 and seasonal flu vaccination hesitancy: Links to personality and general intelligence in a large, UK cohort. Vaccine. (2022) 40:4488–95. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2022.05.062

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Peters E, Joseph S, Day S, Garety P. Measuring delusional ideation: the 21-item Peters et al. Delusions Inventory (PDI). Schizophr Bull. (2004) 30:1005–22. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007116

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Van Os J, Linscott RJ, Myin-Germeys I, Delespaul P, Krabbendam L. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychol Med. (2009) 39:179–95. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003814

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Kuhn S, Lieb R, Freeman D, Andreou C, Zander-Schellenberg T. Coronavirus conspiracy beliefs in the German-speaking general population: endorsement rates and links to reasoning biases and paranoia. Psychol Med. (2021) 1-15. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721001124

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Larsen EM, Donaldson KR, Liew M, Mohanty A. Conspiratorial thinking during COVID-19: the roles of paranoia, delusion-proneness, and intolerance of uncertainty. Front Psychiatry. (2021) 12:698147. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.698147

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Brotherton R, French CC. Belief in conspiracy theories and susceptibility to the conjunction fallacy. Appl Cogn Psychol. (2014) 28:238–48. doi: 10.1002/acp.v28.2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Wabnegger A, Gremsl A, Schienle A. The association between the belief in coronavirus conspiracy theories, miracles, and the susceptibility to conjunction fallacy. Appl Cognit Psychol. (2021) 35:1344–8. doi: 10.1002/acp.v35.5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Murphy J, Vallieres F, Bentall RP, Shevlin M, Mcbride O, Hartman TK, et al. Psychological characteristics associated with COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and resistance in Ireland and the United Kingdom. Nat Commun. (2021) 12:29. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-20226-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Carlton CN, Garcia KM, Andino MV, Ollendick TH, Richey JA. Social anxiety disorder is Associated with Vaccination attitude, stress, and coping responses during COVID-19. Cognit Ther Res. (2022) 46:916–26. doi: 10.1007/s10608-022-10310-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Rosli N, Johar ER, Rosli N, Abdul Hamid NF. Psychological impact of COVID-19: Assessing the COVID-19-related anxiety, individual’s resilience and conspiracy beliefs on attitudes to COVID-19 vaccination. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:906914. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.906914

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Van Prooijen JW, Douglas KM. Belief in conspiracy theories: Basic principles of an emerging research domain. Eur J Soc Psychol. (2018) 48:897–908. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2018.48.issue-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Leibovitz T, Shamblaw AL, Rumas R, Best MW. COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs: Relations with anxiety, quality of life, and schemas. Pers Individ Dif. (2021) 175:110704. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.110704

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Jutzi CA, Willardt R, Schmid PC, Jonas E. Between conspiracy beliefs, ingroup bias, and system justification: how people use defense strategies to cope with the threat of COVID-19. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:578586. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.578586

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Louzolo A, Gustavsson P, Tigerstrom L, Ingvar M, Olsson A, Petrovic P. Delusion-proneness displays comorbidity with traits of autistic-spectrum disorders and ADHD. PloS One. (2017) 12:e0177820. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0177820

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Georgiou N, Delfabbro P, Balzan R. Autistic traits as a potential confounding factor in the relationship between schizotypy and conspiracy beliefs. Cognit Neuropsy. (2021) 26:273–92. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2021.1924650

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Lebedev AV, Acar K, Garzon B, Almeida R, Raback J, Aberg A, et al. Psychedelic drug use and schizotypy in young adults. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:15058. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-94421-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Spielberger CD. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: Bibliography. Palo Alto, CA, USA: Consulting Psychologists Press (1989).

Google Scholar

25. Ritvo RA, Ritvo ER, Guthrie D, Ritvo MJ, Hufnagel DH, Mcmahon W, et al. The Ritvo Autism Asperger Diagnostic Scale-Revised (RAADS-R): a scale to assist the diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder in adults: an international validation study. J Autism Dev Disord. (2011) 41:1076–89. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1133-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Silverstein MJ, Alperin S, Faraone SV, Kessler RC, Adler LA. Test-retest reliability of the adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS) v1.1 Screener in non-ADHD controls from a primar

Comments (0)

No login
gif